Вы и ваши гормоны. Наука о женском здоровье и гормональной контрацепции - Сара Хилл
Шрифт:
Интервал:
Закладка:
29. Schwarz, S., & Hassebrauck, M. (2008). Self-perceived and observed variations in women’s attractiveness throughout the menstrual cycle—a diary study. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(4), 282–288. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.02.003; Röder, S., Brewer, G., & Fink, B. (2009). Menstrual cycle shifts in women’s self-perception and motivation: A daily report method. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(6), 616–619. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.019.
30. Durante, K.M., & Arsena, A.R. (2015). Playing the field: The effect of fertility on women’s desire for variety. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(6), 1372–1391. doi:10.1086/679652.
31. Durante, K.M., Griskevicius, V., Hill, S.E. et al. (2011). Ovulation, female competition, and product choice: Hormonal influences on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 921–934. doi:10.1086/656575; Mortezaie, M., Haselton, M.G., Bleske-Rechek, A. et al. (2007). Ovulatory shifts in ornamentation: Near ovulation, women dress to impress. Hormones and Behavior, 51(1), 40–45. doi:10.1037/e511092014-268; Röder, S., Brewer, G., & Fink, B. (2009). Menstrual cycle shifts in women’s self-perception and motivation: A daily report method. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(6), 616–619. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.019.
32. Guéguen, N. (2012). Makeup and menstrual cycle: Near ovulation, women use more cosmetics. Psychological Record, 62(3), 541–548. doi:10.1007/bf03395819.
33. Durante, K.M., Li, N.P., & Haselton, M.G. (2008). Changes in women’s choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: Naturalistic and laboratory task-based evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(11), 1451–1460. doi:10.1177/014616720832310.
34. Durante, K.M., Griskevicius, V., Hill, S.E. et al. (2011). Ovulation, female competition, and product choice: Hormonal influences on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 921–934. doi:10.1086/656575.
35. Beall, A.T., & Tracy, J.L. (2013). Women are more likely to wear red or pink at peak fertility. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1837–1841. doi:10.1177/0956797613476045; Eisenbruch, A.B., Simmons, Z.L., & Roney, J.R. (2015). Lady in red. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1332–1338. doi:10.1177/0956797615586403.
36. Elliot, A.J., & Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances men’s attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1150–1164. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1150; Elliot, A.J., Tracy, J.L., Pazda, A.D., & Beall, A.T. (2013). Red enhances women’s attractiveness to men: First evidence suggesting universality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 165–168. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.07.017; Kayser, D.N., Elliot, A.J., & Feltman, R. (2010). Red and romantic behavior in men viewing women. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(6), 901–908. doi:10.1002/ejsp.757; Pazda, A.D., Elliot, A.J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2012). Sexy red: Perceived sexual receptivity mediates the red-attraction relation in men viewing woman. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(3), 787–790. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.009; Schwarz S., & Singer, M. (2013). Romantic red revisited: Red enhances men’s attraction to young, but not menopausal women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 161–164. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.08.004.
37. Gangestad, S.W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1399), 927–933. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0380; Gangestad, S.W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C.E. (2005). Adaptations to ovulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 312–316. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00388.x.
38. Gangestad, S.W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1399), 927–933. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0380; Havlicek, J., Roberts, S.C., & Flegr, J. (2005). Women’s preference for dominant male odour: Effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status. Biology Letters, 1(3), 256–259. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0332; Scheib, J.E., Gangestad, S.W., & Thornhill, R. (1999). Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 266(1431), 1913–1917. doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0866.
39. Johnston, V.S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M. et al. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: Evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(4), 251–267. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(01)00066-6; Penton-Voak, I.S., & Perrett, D.I. (2000). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(1), 39–48. doi:10.1016/s1090-5138(99)00033-1; Penton-Voak, I.S., Perrerr, D.I., Castles, D.L. et al. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399, 741–742. doi:10.1038/21557; Roney, J.R., & Simmons, Z.L. (2008). Women’s estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men’s testosterone. Hormones and Behavior, 53(1), 14–19. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.008; Roney, J.R., & Simmons, Z.L. (2008). Women’s estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men’s testosterone. Hormones and Behavior, 53(1), 14–19. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.008.
40. Feinberg, D.R., Debruine, L.M., Jones, B.C., & Little, A.C. (2008). Correlated preferences for men’s facial and vocal masculinity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(4), 233–241. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.008; Puts, D.A. (2006). Cyclic variation in women’s preferences for masculine traits. Human Nature, 17(1), 114–127. doi:10.1007/s12110-006-1023-x.