Книги онлайн и без регистрации » Домашняя » Как мы учимся. Почему мозг учится лучше, чем любая машина… пока - Станислас Деан

Как мы учимся. Почему мозг учится лучше, чем любая машина… пока - Станислас Деан

Шрифт:

-
+

Интервал:

-
+

Закладка:

Сделать
1 ... 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 ... 101
Перейти на страницу:

American Academy of Pediatrics. (2014). School start times for adolescents. Pediatrics, 134(3), 642–649.

Amunts, K., Lenzen, M., Friederici, A. D., Schleicher, A., Morosan, P., Palomero– Gallagher, N., & Zilles, K. (2010). Broca’s region: Novel organizational principles and multiple receptor mapping. PLOS Biology, 8(9), e1000489.

Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2015). Architectonic mapping of the human brain beyond Brodmann. Neuron, 88(6), 1086–1107.

Anderson, R. C., Wilson, P. T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988). Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly, 23(3), 285–303.

Ansari, D., & Dhital, B. (2006). Age-related changes in the activation of the intraparietal sulcus during nonsymbolic magnitude processing: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(11), 1820–1828.

Antony, J. W., Gobel, E. W., O’Hare, J. K., Reber, P. J., & Paller, K. A. (2012). Cued memory reactivation during sleep influences skill learning. Nature Neuroscience, 15(8), 1114–1116.

Arnould, L. (1900). Une âme en prison: Histoire de l’ éducation d’une aveugle-sourde-muette de naissance. Paris: Oudin.

Arzi, A., Shedlesky, L., Ben-Shaul, M., Nasser, K., Oksenberg, A., Hairston, I. S., & Sobel, N. (2012). Humans can learn new information during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 15(10), 1460–1465.

Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 181–185.

Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(2), 366–377.

Auble, P. M., & Franks, J. J. (1978). The effects of effort toward comprehension on recall. Memory and Cognition, 6(1), 20–25.

Auble, P. M., Franks, J. J., & Soraci, S. A. (1979). Effort toward comprehension: Elaboration or «aha»? Memory and Cognition, 7(6), 426–434.

Avior, G., Fishman, G., Leor, A., Sivan, Y., Kaysar, N., & Derowe, A. (2004). The effect of tonsillectomy and adenoidectomy on inattention and impulsivity as measured by the Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA) in children with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Otolaryngology, 131(4), 367–371.

Bahdanau, D., Cho, K., & Bengio, Y. (2014). Neural machine translation by jointly learning to align and translate. arxiv.org/abs/1409.0473.

Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 62(6), 1227–1246.

Baillargeon, R., Needham, A., & DeVos, J. (1992). The development of young infants’ intuitions about support. Early Development and Parenting, 1(2), 69–78.

Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., Bill, B., Desjardins, R. N., Irwin, J. M., & Tidball, G. (1996). Infants’ reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. Child Development, 67(6), 3135–3153.

Balsam, P. D., & Gallistel, C. R. (2009). Temporal maps and informativeness in associative learning. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(2), 73–78.

Banino, A., Barry, C., Uria, B., Blundell, C., Lillicrap, T., Mirowski, P., Kumaran, D. (2018). Vector-based navigation using grid-like representations in artificial agents. Nature, 557(7705), 429–433.

Bao, S., Chan, V. T., & Merzenich, M. M. (2001). Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Nature, 412(6842), 79–83.

Bavelier, D., Green, C. S., Han, D. H., Renshaw, P. F., Merzenich, M. M., & Gentile, D. A. (2011). Brains on video games. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(12), 763–768.

Beckers, T., Miller, R. R., De Houwer, J., & Urushihara, K. (2006). Reasoning rats: Forward blocking in Pavlovian animal conditioning is sensitive to constraints of causal inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 92–102.

Bedny, M. (2017). Evidence from blindness for a cognitively pluripotent cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 637–648.

Bedny, M., Pascual-Leone, A., Dodell-Feder, D., Fedorenko, E., & Saxe, R. (2011). Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(11), 4429–4434.

Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 328–337.

Bekinschtein, T. A., Dehaene, S., Rohaut, B., Tadel, F., Cohen, L., & Naccache, L. (2009). Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(5), 1672–1677.

Belle, M., Godefroy, D., Couly, G., Malone, S. A., Collier, F., Giacobini, P., & Chédotal, A. (2017). Tridimensional visualization and analysis of early human development. Cell, 169(1), 161–173.

Bendor, D., & Wilson, M. A. (2012). Biasing the content of hippocampal replay during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 15(10), 1439–1444.

Berens, A. E., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). The science of early adversity: Is there a role for large institutions in the care of vulnerable children? Lancet, 386(9991), 388–398.

Bergman-Nutley, S., & Klingberg, T. (2014). Effect of working memory training on working memory, arithmetic and following instructions. Psychological Research, 78(6), 869–877.

Berkes, P., Orbán, G., Lengyel, M., & Fiser, J. (2011). Spontaneous cortical activity reveals hallmarks of an optimal internal model of the environment. Science, 331(6013), 83–87.

Bermudez, P., Lerch, J. P., Evans, A. C., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). Neuroanatomical correlates of musicianship as revealed by cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry. Cerebral Cortex, 19(7), 1583–1596.

Bernal, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Millotte, S., & Christophe, A. (2010). Two-year-olds compute syntactic structure on-line. Developmental Science, 13(1), 69–76.

Bessa, C., Maciel, P., & Rodrigues, A. J. (2013). Using C. elegans to decipher the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying neurodevelopmental disorders. Molecular Neurobiology, 48(3), 465–489.

Bevins, R. A. (2001). Novelty seeking and reward: Implications for the study of high-risk behaviors. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(6), 189–193.

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(3), 89–129.

Binder, J. R., Medler, D. A., Westbury, C. F., Liebenthal, E., & Buchanan, L. (2006). Tuning of the human left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic structure. NeuroImage, 33(2), 739–748.

1 ... 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 ... 101
Перейти на страницу:

Комментарии
Минимальная длина комментария - 20 знаков. В коментария нецензурная лексика и оскорбления ЗАПРЕЩЕНЫ! Уважайте себя и других!
Комментариев еще нет. Хотите быть первым?